Research Articles

Do Romantic Relationships Promote Happiness? Relationships’ Characteristics as Predictors of Subjective Well-Being

Amanda Londero-Santos*1, Jean Carlos Natividade2, Terezinha Féres-Carneiro2

Interpersona, 2021, Vol. 15(1), 3–19, https://doi.org/10.5964/ijpr.4195

Received: 2020-08-11. Accepted: 2021-02-20. Published (VoR): 2021-06-30.

*Corresponding author at: Departament of Psychology, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Rua Marques de São Vicente, 225. Gávea. Rio de Janeiro-RJ, Brasil. CEP: 22451-900. E-mail: amandalondero@me.com

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

This study aimed to investigate the predictive power of aspects of the romantic relationship on subjective well-being, beyond what is explained by sociodemographic and personality variables. Participants were 490 heterosexual adults (68.8% women), all involved in a monogamous romantic relationship. Romantic relationship variables were substantial predictors of the three components of subjective well-being, explaining 21% of the variance in life satisfaction, 19% of the variance in positive affect, and 15% of the variance in negative affect, in addition to sociodemographic variables and personality factors. Still, relationship satisfaction was one of the main predictors of subjective well-being. The results highlight the importance of romantic relationships over subjective well-being, suggesting that cultivating satisfying romantic relationships contributes to a happier life.

Keywords: romantic relationship, romantic satisfaction, personality, subjective well-being

Subjective well-being (SWB) generally refers to “the extent to which a person believes or feels that his or her life is doing well” (Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2018, p. 2) and often involves three components: life satisfaction, positive affect and negative affect. SWB is greater the higher the life satisfaction, the more positive affect and the less negative affect the person experiences. Affects are generic terms that encompass a large number of sensations experienced by people, including emotions and feelings. Positive affect (experiencing positive emotions and feelings) and negative affect (experiencing negative emotions and feelings) are affective components of subjective well-being. Life satisfaction, the cognitive component of subjective well-being, refers to a person’s assessment of his or her life and can be considered an attitude to one’s own life (Schimmack, 2008).

Numerous studies has been conducted in all parts of the world to investigate possible predictors of SWB (for a review see Diener, Oishi, & Tay, 2018a; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). They suggest multiple factors (genetic and environmental) as responsible for explaining the variance of SWB components. Important predictors found are sociodemographic variables (e.g., gender, age, financial status, religion, having children), personality factors (e.g., neuroticism, extroversion), and romantic relationship-related variables (for a review, see Diener et al., 1999; Diener et al., 2018a). Although studies point to both individual (e.g., personality) and contextual (e.g., romantic relationship-related) characteristics as predictors of SWB, it is important to analyze these variables together, assessing the importance of each in predicting the SWB.

Demographic Variables and SWB

Among the demographic variables associated with SWB, gender, age, financial status, and religion are frequently studied. However, for some of these (e.g., gender and age), the results are often divergent from one another. Even in researches with large representative population samples or in meta-analyses, findings on gender differences in SWB are inconsistent (for a review, see Batz & Tay, 2018). Also, regarding age, contrasting findings are not uncommon (for a review, see Lansford, 2018). Studies have found a U-shaped relationship between well-being and age, characterized by higher levels of well-being at the beginning and end of adulthood, but this form of relationship is most often reported in research in countries with high economic development. In developing countries, however, the most regularly found relationship throughout adulthood is a decreasing linear relationship (e.g., González Gutiérrez, Jiménez, Hernández, & Puente, 2005; Steptoe, Deaton, & Stone, 2015).

Having children is another demographic variable that seems to impact subjective well-being; however, findings are often conflicting. Nelson-Coffey (2018) reports that the relationship between parenthood and subjective well-being is complex and dynamic. However, in general, longitudinal studies have found an increase in well-being after having children, followed by a decline. Still, other demographic variables (e.g., relationship status, economic status) appear to mediate the relationship between having children and SWB (e.g., Angeles, 2010; for a review, see Nelson-Coffey, 2018).

With respect to other demographic variables (e.g., economic resources and religiosity), the results have been more uniform. Economic resources are consistently positively associated with SWB (e.g., Angeles, 2010; Diener, Tay, & Oishi, 2013; Stevenson & Wolfers, 2013; for a review, see Mogilner, Whillans, & Norton, 2018). The availability of economic resources seems to contribute to increasing subjective well-being as they can be employed to improve quality of life (e.g., better health care, investing in leisure time, buying objects that benefit life; see Mogilner et al., 2018). In fact, the relationship between income and well-being is stronger in poorer countries (with worse living conditions) compared to richer countries (see Diener, Oishi, & Tay, 2018b). Regarding religiosity, studies have found evidence that religious people have higher levels of SWB than non-religious people (e.g., Tay, Li, Myers, & Diener, 2014; for a review, see Kim-Prieto & Miller, 2018). Although it is not yet clear how the relationship between religiosity and SWB takes place (for a review, see Newman & Graham, 2018).

Personality Factors and SWB

Although the relationship between demographic variables and SWB has been widely investigated, they explain only a small amount of SWB variance (Diener et al., 1999; see Nes & Roysamb, 2015). The most consistent and important predictors of SWB seem to be personality factors (for a review, see Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2018; Lucas, 2018). The association between SWB and personality factors is one of the most consistent and robust findings found in the literature. In a meta-analysis, DeNeve and Cooper (1998) found that, compared to the other personality factors, neuroticism was the factor with the highest correlation, either with life satisfaction (r = -.24) or with negative affect (r = -.23). Extroversion and agreeableness were the factors that showed the highest correlation with positive affect (r = .20; r = .17, respectively; DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). Longitudinal studies revealed that the main predictor of the components of subjective well-being is neuroticism; however, extroversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness were important predictors of the three components of SWB (Soto, 2015).

Romantic Relationship Variables and SWB

Romantic relationship variables have also been associated with SWB, especially relationship status. For example, studies have found that married people have higher levels of subjective well-being than other marital statuses (e.g., Argyle, 1999; Diener, Gohm, Suh, & Oishi, 2000; Myers, 1999). However, these findings should be carefully analyzed. Because of hedonic adaptation, life satisfaction after marriage tends to return to premarital levels (Stutzer & Frey, 2006; see Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2018). Still, the higher SWB levels of married people seem to stem partly from the fact that people with high levels of subjective well-being most often marry and stay in their relationships. People with low levels of subjective well-being remain single or divorce (Lucas, 2007; Stutzer & Frey, 2006)

Faced with the frequency of consensual unions (without marriage) in western cultures, researchers have investigated differences in well-being between people married and those in a consensual union. Diener et al. (2000) found that married people have higher levels of subjective well-being than cohabiting people. However, this association between relationship status and well-being appears to be controlled by other variables. For example, after controlling for the effect of participant selection biases (characteristics that are associated with the propensity to marry, such as parental divorce and parental financial income) and relationship satisfaction, Perelli-Harris, Hoherz, Lappegård, and Evans (2019) found no differences between married people and those in consensual union (cohabitants).

The length of the romantic relationship can also be a factor that impacts subjective well-being. However, few studies investigated the impact of relationship length and SWB, and there is no consensus on the relationship between these variables. While Ventegodt, Flensborg-Madsen, Andersen, and Merrick (2008) found a weak but significant association between relationship length and quality of life, Rhoades, Kamp Dush, Atkins, Stanley, and Markman (2011) reported that relationship duration was not a significant predictor of either life satisfaction or psychological distress after the couple’s separation. Also, studies that examined the association between relationship length and marital satisfaction have shown a decreasing linear relationship between these variables (for a review, see Karney, 2015).

People who are in happy relationships report high levels of subjective well-being (Kamp Dush & Amato, 2005). A meta-analysis showed that quality of the romantic relationship is significantly associated with well-being (Proulx, Helms, & Buehler, 2007). Researchers have consistently detected an association between relationship satisfaction and SWB (e.g., Cao, Wang, & Wang, 2020; Love & Holder, 2016; Londero-Santos, Pereira Neto, & Natividade, 2017). Besides that, a longitudinal study suggests that relationship satisfaction has a causal influence on life satisfaction (e.g., Headey, Veenhoven, & Wearing, 1991). Other studies also highlight the increased importance of romantic relationships on people’s well-being, as evidenced by the fact that in recent years there has been an increase in the association between relationship satisfaction and well-being indicators (see Braithwaite & Holt-Lunstad, 2017; Finkel, Cheung, Emery, Carswell, & Larson, 2015).

A possible explanation for the observed relationships between relationship satisfaction and SWB may be given by theories using bottom-up processing models (see Schimmack, 2008). Bottom-up processing models suggest that subjective well-being derives from a sum of pleasant and unpleasant moments and experiences. The satisfaction of goals theories, which are based on these processing models, for example, asserts that satisfaction with one’s own life stems from the satisfaction of one’s own needs, desires, and goals (see Diener et al., 1999; Diener et al., 2018b). Good romantic relationships would therefore satisfy the basic needs, desires, and goals of individuals. Diener, Scollon, and Lucas (2009) assume that if it were possible to access all domains of a person’s life, it would be possible to infer that person’s assessment of his/her own life (life satisfaction) using bottom-up processing. Thus, the attitude to life itself (i.e., life satisfaction) derives from attitudes towards specific aspects of life (for a review, see Schimmack, 2008). The individual, therefore, would be more satisfied with his/her own life, the more positive attitudes he/she has about specific aspects of his/her life (e.g., satisfaction with his/her romantic relationship).

Thus, the greater the accessibility and intensity of attitudes of specific aspects of life, the more these aspects will be used to assess satisfaction with one’s own life (Schwarz & Strack, 1999). Indeed, Diener et al. (2009) state that the assessment of life satisfaction stems from information that is salient in the individual’s memory at the time of the assessment. Diener et al. (2009) included in the definition of subjective well-being satisfaction with specific aspects of life. Therefore, satisfaction with a romantic relationship should be one of those specific domains that would contribute to assessing the overall aspect of life satisfaction.

Many variables explain variations in well-being levels, and there is no consensus on all those that best predict SWB. Besides, culture plays a critical role in SWB (see Suh & Choi, 2018). Those open questions emphasize the importance of research in different cultures to provide a complete understanding of the factors that impact well-being. Results from studies in different cultures may allow the identification of specific and universal SWB predictors. Therefore, this study was developed to investigate the predictive power of romantic relationship variables on subjective well-being beyond what is explained by sociodemographic and personality variables.

Method

Participants

Participants were 490 heterosexual adults, with a mean age of 33.9 years (SD = 9.66), ranging from 18 to 69 years. Most participants were women, 68.8% (n = 337). All participants were involved in a monogamous romantic relationship. The mean relationship length was 9.45 years (SD = 8.67). Most participants were married, 45.5%; 27.1% of them lived in consensual union without marriage (with or without legal recognition), and the remaining 27.4% were in a committed relationship or engaged. Most participants, 73.9% (n = 362), reported cohabiting with their spouse or partner. Of the total participants, 33.1% reported having children.

Most participants lived in the southern (41.4%) and southeast (37.1%) regions of Brazil, the others were from the northeast (11.6%), central west (6.1%), northern (1.0%), and the others were outside Brazil when they responded the questionnaire (2.7%). Regarding education, 42% participants reported having completed graduate studies (in particular, 17.3% higher education specialization, 17.6% master degree, and 7.1% doctoral degree). Other 22.8% of participants were attending or attended (without having completed) graduate studies; 20.0% of the participants reported having just completed higher education; 14.1% said they had incomplete higher education, and 1.1% reported having just completed high school. The mean of monthly per capita income of the participants was R$ 4,098.17 (SD = 3,508.23), approximately US$ 1,280.00. Regarding religious practice, 36.5% declared to have no religion, 29.4% declared to be a non-practicing religious, and 34.1% declared to be religious practicing people.

Instruments

Data were collected through an online questionnaire available on the internet containing sociodemographic questions (e.g., gender, sexual orientation, education, relationship length, religiosity, family income, cohabitation of partner with participant). The questionnaire also contained scales to measure relationship satisfaction, personality and subjective well-being (positive and negative affect, and life satisfaction).

Romantic Relationship Satisfaction Level Scale – Revised (RRLS-R)

Relationship satisfaction was measured using the RRLS-R (Londero-Santos, Natividade, & Féres-Carneiro, 2021). RRLS-R is a revised version of the Rusbult, Martz, and Agnew (1998) scale. The instrument contains five affirmative items to be answered on a nine-point agreement scale, such that 0 = Strongly Disagree and 8 = Strongly Agree, the higher the scores, the greater the satisfaction with the romantic relationship. Londero-Santos et al. (2021) found evidence of validity based on internal structure, on relations to other variables, and convergent validity. Also, an item response theory analysis shown adequate parameters for the items, the items were highly discriminatory and covering a large extent of the latent construct. The scale alpha coefficient was .91 (Londero-Santos et al., 2021). In this study, the total omega reliability coefficient was .91.

Personality Descriptors Scale – Reduced (PDS-R)

PDS-R (Natividade & Hutz, 2015) assesses the big five personality factors: extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness. The scale is made up of 20 adjectives or small expressions, four for each factor; so that participants answer how much they agree that each one describes them properly, on a seven-point scale. The higher the participants’ scores on each factor, the higher the intensity of the factors. In the study of Natividade and Hutz (2015), the scale presented adequate evidence of validity based on internal structure, relations to other variables, and convergent validity. Also, alpha coefficients ranged from .59 to .84 among the factors, and test-retest correlations ranged from .69 to .81 (Natividade & Hutz, 2015). In this study, the total omega reliability coefficient ranged from .65 to .87 among the factors.

Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS)

PANAS (Zanon & Hutz, 2014) is formed by two orthogonal factors, positive affect and negative affect, and it is an adaptation of the scale originally proposed by Watson and Clark (1994). Each factor consists of 10 adjectives that represent the subjects’ emotions and moods. The intensity of affects is judged by the participants on a five-point scale. In this study, the total omega reliability coefficient was .90 for Negative Affect and .88 for Positive Affect.

Life Satisfaction Scale

Life Satisfaction Scale (Hutz, Zanon, & Bardagi, 2014) — an adaptation of the satisfaction with life scale, originally proposed by Diener, Emmons, Larsen, and Griffin (1985). This scale consists of five items that globally assess the cognitive aspects of subjective well-being. Items are answered on a seven-point Likert scale. The scale presented adequate internal consistency, with an alpha coefficient of .87, in addition to presenting scalar invariance across gender (Zanon, Bardagi, Layous, & Hutz, 2014). In this study, the total omega reliability coefficient was .88.

The questionnaire had different versions so that words, when necessary, were flexed according to the gender of the participant and his/her respective partner, in order to avoid bias in the answer (Natividade, Barros, & Hutz, 2012). That is important because in the Portuguese language some adjectives are flexed according to gender, and that could influence responses from men and women. Also, along with the scale items, there were control questions to check the fidelity of the items’ answers.

Procedures

Data Collection

Participants were recruited through email invitations and social networking sites. The invitation emails were sent to a list of individuals who had previously agreed to participate in our laboratory research. Concerning social networks, invitations were posted on the researchers’ websites, research authors’ laboratory websites, and social scientific research-related-websites. Those who accepted to participate had to click on the address link provided in the invitation and were directed to the questionnaire. This research was sent to the Ethics Committee on Human Research and obtained a favorable agreement.

Data Analysis

Data cleaning was performed, excluding incorrect answers to control questions. Then, data scatter plots were constructed in order to check the distribution of variables visually. As they had substantial negative asymmetry, the variables per capita income and relationship length were transformed into logarithms. The dichotomous variables gender, having children, relationship status (consensual union, married, cohabitation) were transformed into dummy variables of 0 (being male; having no children; no living in a consensual union, no married, no cohabitation with a romantic partner) and 1 (being female; having children; living in a consensual union, married, cohabitation with a romantic partner). The religious practice variable was coded as 0 = no religion, 1 = religious non-practicing, and 2 = religious practicing. Subsequently, the correlation coefficients between the variables were calculated. Finally, we tested the predictive power of romantic relationship-related variables (relationship length, relationship status, and relationship satisfaction) on subjective well-being by controlling the effects of sociodemographic variables (gender, age, per capita income, and having children) and the big five factors through multiple hierarchical regressions (Enter method). Only the variables that showed a statistically significant correlation with the predicted variable were included in the regression models. Preliminary analyses were conducted to ensure no violation of the assumptions of normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homoscedasticity.

Results

Initially, we calculated Pearson’s correlation coefficients, as can be seen in Table 1. In the regression analyses, we included those variables that significantly correlated with the factors of subjective well-being. Thereby, we investigated the predictive power of variables concerning the romantic relationship on subjective well-being, after controlling for the influence of sociodemographic and personality variables. To this end, three hierarchical regression analyses were run, one for each component of subjective well-being, which included sociodemographic variables in the first block, personality factors in the second block, and romantic relationship variables in the third block. Results can be seen in Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4.

Table 1

Correlation Between Subjective Well-being, Sociodemographic Variables, Personality, and Romantic Relationship

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
1. Life satisfaction (.87)
2. Positive affect .59*** (.89)
3. Negative affect -.46*** -.51*** (.90)
4. Gender -.07 -.02 .01
5. Age -.01 -.03 .01 .13**
6. Per capita income (log) .18*** -.001 -.02 .05 .25***
7. Religious practice .11* .15** -.10* -.15** .12** -.04
8. Having children -.04 .04 .01 -.01 .57*** -.02 .18***
9. Extroversion .09* .25*** -.08 -.14** .05 .01 .15** .12** (.84)
10. Agreeableness .13** .24*** -.12** -.06 -.03 -.06 .11* .02 .45*** (.78)
11. Neuroticism -.30*** -.24*** .46*** -.22*** -.16*** -.02 .001 -.06 .10* -.16*** (.68)
12. Conscientiousness .17*** .25*** -.14** -.24*** -.02 -.01 .14** -.01 .10* .21*** -.12** (.72)
13. Openness .10* .22*** -.07 .02 .07 -.05 -.12** .03 .32*** .32*** -.09 .09* (.60)
14. Relat satisfaction .51*** .47*** -.45*** .01 -.03 .09 .04 -.09* .02 .004 -.15** .04 -.03 (.91)
15. Relat length (log) -.02 -.06 .04 -.002 .59*** .10* .11* .49*** -.01 -.09 -.03 .00 -.04 -.07
16. Consensual union -.11* -.09* .12* -.07 .03 .04 -.09* -.05 .03 -.06 .12** -.08 .10* -.18*** .003
17. Married .12** .08 -.06 -.02 .33*** .12** .15*** .39*** .003 -.02 -.09* .05 -.05 .13** .50*** -.56***
18. Cohabitation .04 -.002 .03 -.08 .40*** .18*** .05 .39*** .03 -.11* .01 -.03 .05 -.02 .56*** .33*** .54***

Note. Per capita income (log) = Logarithm of per capita income; Relat satisfaction = Relationship satisfaction; Relat length (log) = Logarithm of relationship length. Gender: 0 = man; 1 = woman. For variables, having children, consensual union, married and cohabitation: 0 = no; 1 = yes. In parentheses on the main diagonal are the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the scales. N = 490.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 2

Hierarchical Regression Results for Life Satisfaction

Variable B 95% CI for B
SE B β R2 Adjusted R2 R 2
LL UL
Step 1 .05 .04 .05***
Constant 2.57*** 1.52 3.63 0.54
Per capita income (log) 0.63*** 0.33 0.92 0.15 .18***
Religious practice 0.18** 0.04 0.31 0.07 .12**
Step 2 .16 .15 .12***
Constant 2.30** 0.86 3.74 0.73
Per capita income (log) 0.62*** 0.34 0.90 0.14 .18***
Religious practice 0.15* 0.02 0.28 0.07 .10*
Extroversion 0.06 -0.03 0.15 0.04 .06
Agreeableness 0.02 -0.11 0.15 0.07 .02
Neuroticism -0.31*** -0.41 -0.22 0.05 -.29***
Conscientiousness 0.13* 0.03 0.23 0.05 .11*
Openness 0.08 -0.04 0.20 0.06 .06
Step 3 .37 .36 .21***
Constant 0.79 -0.49 2.07 0.65
Per capita income (log) 0.49*** 0.24 0.74 0.13 .14***
Religious practice 0.13* 0.01 0.24 0.06 .08*
Extroversion 0.03 -0.05 0.11 0.04 .03
Agreeableness 0.05 -0.07 0.16 0.06 .03
Neuroticism -0.23*** -0.32 -0.15 0.04 -.21***
Conscientiousness 0.11* 0.03 0.20 0.04 .10*
Openness 0.10* <0.01 0.21 0.05 .08*
Consensual union 0.04 -0.21 0.30 0.13 .02
Married 0.05 -0.17 0.28 0.12 .02
Relationship satisfaction 0.31*** 0.26 0.36 0.02 .46***

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. Religious practice: 0 = no religion; 1 = religious non-practicing; 2 = religious practicing. Consensual union and married: 0 = no; 1 = yes. N = 490.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 3

Hierarchical Regression Results for Positive Affect

Variable B 95% CI for B
SE B β R2 Adjusted R2 R 2
LL UL
Step 1 .02 .02 .02**
Constant 3.18*** 3.08 3.28 0.05
Religious practice 0.13** 0.05 0.20 0.04 .15**
Step 2 .19 .18 .17***
Constant 2.05*** 1.51 2.60 0.28
Religious practice 0.10** 0.03 0.17 0.04 .11**
Extroversion 0.09*** 0.04 0.14 0.02 .18***
Agreeableness 0.02 -0.05 0.10 0.04 .03
Neuroticism -0.14*** -0.19 -0.08 0.03 -.22***
Conscientiousness 0.11*** 0.06 0.17 0.03 .17***
Openness 0.10** 0.03 0.16 0.03 .14**
Step 3 .38 .37 .19***
Constant 0.99*** 0.48 1.50 0.26
Religious practice 0.09** 0.03 0.15 0.03 .10**
Extroversion 0.07*** 0.03 0.12 0.02 .15***
Agreeableness 0.04 -0.03 0.10 0.03 .05
Neuroticism -0.09*** -0.14 -0.05 0.02 -.15***
Conscientiousness 0.10*** 0.06 0.15 0.02 .16***
Openness 0.11*** 0.06 0.17 0.03 .16***
Consensual union 0.02 -0.10 0.13 0.06 .01
Relationship satisfaction 0.16*** 0.14 0.19 0.01 .44***

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. Religious practice: 0 = no religion; 1 = religious non-practicing; 2 = religious practicing. Consensual union: 0 = no; 1 = yes. N = 490.

**p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 4

Hierarchical Regression Results for Negative Affect

Variable B 95% CI for B
SE B β R2 Adjusted R2 R 2
LL UL
Step 1 .10 .008 .10*
Constant 2.21*** 2.11 2.32 0.05
Religious practice -0.09* -0.18 -0.01 0.04 -.10*
Step 2 .23 .22 .22***
Constant 1.47*** 0.94 2.01 0.27
Religious practice -0.08* -0.16 -0.01 0.04 -.09*
Agreeableness -0.02 -0.09 0.04 0.03 -.03
Neuroticism 0.30*** 0.25 0.36 0.03 .45***
Conscientiousness -0.05 -0.11 0.01 0.03 -.06
Step 3 .38 .37 .15***
Constant 2.46*** 1.94 2.98 0.26
Religious practice -0.07* -0.14 0.00 0.03 -.08*
Agreeableness -0.03 -0.09 0.03 0.03 -.04
Neuroticism 0.26*** 0.21 0.31 0.02 .39***
Conscientiousness -0.04 -0.09 0.01 0.03 -.06
Consensual union -0.03 -0.16 0.10 0.07 -.02
Relationship satisfaction -0.16*** -0.19 -0.13 0.01 -.39***

Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. Religious practice: 0 = no religion; 1 = religious non-practicing; 2 = religious practicing. Consensual union: 0 = no; 1 = yes. N = 490.

*p < .05. ***p < .001.

Life Satisfaction

Logarithm of per capita income and religious practice were entered at Step 1, explaining 4.52% of the variance in life satisfaction, F(2, 487) = 11.54, p < .001. When entered into Step 2, extroversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, conscientiousness, and openness resulted in a significant increase in the variance of intentions, R2change = .12, Fchange = (5, 482) = 13.42, p < .001, and the entire model remained significant, F(7, 482) = 13.30, p < .001. All the sociodemographic constructs remained significant at Step 2, along with the personality constructs of neuroticism and conscientiousness. After entry of consensual union, married, and relationship satisfaction, at Step 3, the total variance explained by the model as a whole was 37%, F(10, 479) = 27.97, p < .001. The relationship variables explained an additional 21% of the variance in life satisfaction, R2change = .21, Fchange(3, 479) = 52.29, p < . 001. In the final model, per capita income, religious practice, neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, and relationship satisfaction were statistically significant (see Table 2). Relationship satisfaction (β = .46) explained the most unique variance in life satisfaction, followed by neuroticism (β = -.21).

Positive Affect

Religious practice, at Step 1, significantly accounted for 2.19% of the variance in positive affect, F(1, 488) = 10.92, p = .001. When entered into Step 2, extroversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, conscientiousness, and openness resulted in a significant increase in the variance of positive affect, R2change = .17, Fchange = (5, 483) = 20.26, p < .001, and the entire model remained significant, F(6, 483) = 19.06, p < .001. At Step 2, religious practice remained significant predictor of positive affect. For the personality, extroversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, and openness was significant predictors of positive affect (see Table 3). After entry of consensual union and relationship satisfaction at Step 3 the total variance explained by the model as a whole was 38%, F(8, 481) = 36.83, p < .001. The relationship variables explained an additional 18.8% of the variance in positive affect, R2change = .19, Fchange(2, 481) = 73.08, p < .001. In the final model, religious practice, extroversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness, and relationship satisfaction were statistically significant. Relationship satisfaction (β = .44) explained the most unique variance in positive affect, followed by openness (β = .16) and conscientiousness (β = .16).

Negative Affect

Religious practice, at Step 1, significantly accounted for 10.1% of the variance in positive affect, F(1, 488) = 5.02, p = .026. When entered into Step 2, agreeableness, neuroticism, and conscientiousness resulted in a significant increase in the variance of positive affect, R2change = .22, Fchange = (3, 485) = 45.97, p < .001, and the entire model remained significant, F(4, 485) = 36.08, p < .001. At Step 2, the religious practice remained a significant predictor of negative affect. For the personality, only neuroticism was significant predictors of negative affect (see Table 4). After entry of consensual union and relationship satisfaction at Step 3 the total variance explained by the model as a whole was 38%, F(6, 483) = 49.25, p < .001. The relationship variables explained an additional 15% of the variance in positive affect, R2change = .15, Fchange(2, 483) = 58.48, p < .001. In the final model, religious practice, neuroticism, and relationship satisfaction were statistically significant. Relationship satisfaction (β = -.39) and neuroticism (β = .39) explained the most unique variance in negative affect.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the predictive power of variables concerning romantic relationship, such as length of the relationship, relationship status (consensual union, married, cohabitation with a partner), and satisfaction with the romantic relationship, on subjective well-being, beyond what is explained by sociodemographic (gender, age, per capita income, religious practice and having children) and personality (extroversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, conscientiousness, openness to experience) variables. The results revealed that the romantic relationship variables are important predictors of SWB, adding explanation beyond the sociodemographic and personality variables.

However, neither relationship length nor relationship status (consensual union, marriage, cohabitation) impacted SWB, although previous studies have shown an association between relationship status (e.g., married) and life satisfaction (e.g., Lucas, 2007; Luhmann, Hofmann, Eid, & Lucas, 2012). Among the variables of the romantic relationship, the only significant predictor of subjective well-being was relationship satisfaction. Comparing the standardized weights of all predictors SWB included in this study, relationship satisfaction had the highest explanatory power (β = .46 for life satisfaction; β = .44, for positive affect; β = -.39 for negative affect). The other variables concerning the romantic relationship may have diminished their weight, given the importance of relationship satisfaction for well-being. This result highlights the importance of relationship satisfaction in people’s lives.

This study’s findings of the impact of relationship satisfaction on SWB are consistent with other researches (e.g., Hudson, Lucas, & Donnellan, 2020; Londero-Santos et al., 2017; Margelisch, Schneewind, Violette, & Perrig-Chiello, 2017). Although we have not found other studies that have investigated the predictive power of relationship satisfaction on SWB, after controlling for the effect of sociodemographic and personality variables, the present study revealed that relationship satisfaction is a crucial variable to predict subjective well-being.

These results suggest that to evaluate their own life, individuals rely on assessments of specific aspects of life, in this case, the romantic relationship. This provides additional evidence for theories based on bottom-up processing models, which assume that life situations and events influence subjective well-being (see Diener et al., 1999; Diener et al., 2009; Diener et al., 2018b; Schimmack, 2008). Maintaining a positive attitude towards the romantic relationship (satisfaction with the romantic relationship) therefore seems to positively impact the attitude towards one’s own life (life satisfaction). The results suggest that attitudes towards romantic relationships are taken into account when forming attitudes toward life itself. Nevertheless, it should be emphasized that this is a cross-sectional study, making it impossible to identify the causal relationship of these variables. Still, it is possible that the relationship between SWB and relationship satisfaction may have occurred due to the moderation/mediation of other variables that were not considered in this study or that were both determined by other variables. Longitudinal studies may be conducted to investigate the variation in SWB and relationship satisfaction over time.

In addition to relationship satisfaction, sociodemographic and personality variables also predict subjective well-being. The results suggest that sociodemographic variables predict life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect from the tested models. Per capita income was a positive predictor of life satisfaction. This result is in line with previous studies (e.g., Diener et al., 2013; Stevenson & Wolfers, 2013). One possible explanation for the positive effects of per capita income on life satisfaction is that economic resources make it possible for people to improve their lives (see Diener et al., 2018b).

Religious practice was also a positive predictor of life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect. The more involved with religion, the higher the subjective well-being levels of the participants. This result supports other studies that found a positive relationship between religiosity and subjective well-being (e.g., Fleury et al., 2018; for a review, see Diener et al., 1999; Kim-Prieto & Miller, 2018). Religious people tend to have a greater sense of life and better-coping strategies, and religious practice can be an important source of social support, i.e., being involved in religious practice, especially if done in a group context, would increase social contact, and the availability of social support received (for a review, see Kim-Prieto & Miller, 2018).

Regarding personality traits, life satisfaction was predicted negatively by neuroticism and positively by conscientiousness and openness. Positive affect was predicted positively by extroversion, conscientiousness, and openness, and negatively by neuroticism. Finally, negative affect was predicted positively by neuroticism. Among personality factors, neuroticism was the main explanatory variable for life satisfaction and negative affect, consistent with that found in the meta-analysis of DeNeve and Cooper (1998). Thus, having high levels of neuroticism seems to predispose the individual to poorly evaluate his own life and experience more negative affections (the negative correlations between neuroticism and relationship satisfaction also exemplify this).

Extroversion was an important predictor of positive affect, which is convergent with many other research results (e.g., Anglim & Grant, 2016; Natividade, Carvalho, Londero-Santos, Carvalho, Santos, & Fagundes, 2019; Noronha, Lamas, & Barros, 2016). Costa and McCrae (1980) claim that high levels of extroversion would predispose individuals to experience positive affects in their daily lives. Still, there is evidence that extroversion and positive affect are based on the same neurological structure (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991). These findings broadly corroborate other studies that investigated the relationship of the big five personality factors and subjective well-being (e.g., DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; González Gutiérrez et al., 2005; Noronha, Martins, Campos, & Mansão, 2015; Natividade et al., 2019; Soto, 2015).

The results of this study also showed the importance of the factors conscientiousness and openness in the prediction of life satisfaction and positive affect. These two factors are rarely studied as predictors of SWB (Lucas, 2018), and the results are not consistent (e.g., Anglim & Grant, 2016; González Gutiérrez et al., 2005; Malkoç, 2011; Noronha et al., 2016; Natividade et al., 2019). The divergent results can be explained by the different set of variables used in the regression analyzes and the different instruments used to assess the five personality factors (each instrument may evaluate different aspects of personality factors). Therefore, more studies should be carried out to investigate the impact of personality on the SWB in Brazilian samples.

In addition, in this research, only linear relationships between the study variables were investigated. Variables that did not show a statistically significant correlation with the predicted variable were not included in the regression analysis. These variables may have a non-linear relationship with the variables of SWB. Studies have found a positive quadratic relationship between age and well-being (for a review, see Lansford, 2018). Therefore, studies should be conducted to investigate the non-linear relationship of variables with subjective well-being.

Given the above, our results showed important predictors of SWB, especially personality factors and relationship satisfaction. It can be concluded that, from the results of this study, being satisfied with one’s own romantic relationship proved beneficial for the subjective well-being of the individual. Still, SWB seems to reflect the assessment of important aspects of life, such as romantic relationships. Therefore, it is important to cultivate healthy and happy romantic relationships to live a happier and more fulfilling life. These findings may support public policy and clinical practice, as they suggest that helping people improve their romantic relationships can positively impact subjective well-being.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no additional (i.e., non-financial) support to report.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

References

  • Angeles, L. (2010). Children and life satisfaction. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(4), 523-538. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9168-z

  • Anglim, J., & Grant, S. (2016). Predicting psychological and subjective well-being from personality: Incremental prediction from 30 facets over the Big 5. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 59-80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9583-7

  • Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 353–373). New York, NY, USA: Russell Sage Foundation.

  • Batz, C., & Tay, L. (2018). Gender differences in subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 371–385). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Braithwaite, S., & Holt-Lunstad, J. (2017). Romantic relationships and mental health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 120-125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.001

  • Cao, X., Wang, D., & Wang, Y. (2020). Remembering the past and imagining the future: Partners’ responsiveness in specific events relates to relationship satisfaction and subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(2), 538-558. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407519873041

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(4), 668-678. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.4.668

  • DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 197-229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71-75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

  • Diener, E., Gohm, C. L., Suh, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Similarity of the relations between marital status and subjective well-being across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(4), 419-436. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022100031004001

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra. Psychology, 4(1), Article 15. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115

  • Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (Eds.). (2018a). Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (2018b). Advances in subjective well-being research. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(4), 253-260. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6

  • Diener, E., Scollon, C., & Lucas, R. E. (2009) The evolving concept of subjective well-being: The multifaceted nature of happiness. In Diener E. (Ed.), Assessing well-being: Social Indicators Research Series, (Vol. 39, pp. 67-100). New York, NY, USA: Springer.

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276-302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

  • Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267-276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030487

  • Finkel, E. J., Cheung, E. O., Emery, L. F., Carswell, K. L., & Larson, G. M. (2015). The suffocation model why marriage in America is becoming an all-or-nothing institution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 238-244. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415569274

  • Fleury, L. F. O., Gomes, A. M. T., Rocha, J. C. C., Formiga, N. S., Souza, M. M. T., Marques, S. C., & Bernardes, M. M. R. (2018). Religiosidade, estratégias de coping e satisfação com a vida: Verificação de um modelo de influência em estudantes universitários [Religiosity, coping strategies and satisfaction with life: Verifi- cation of a model of influence in university students]. Revista Portuguesa de Enfermagem de Saúde Mental, 20, 51-57. https://doi.org/10.19131/rpesm.0226

  • González Gutiérrez, J. L., Jiménez, B. M., Hernández, E. G., & Puente, C. P. (2005). Personality and subjective well-being: Big five correlates and demographic variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(7), 1561-1569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.015

  • Headey, B., Veenhoven, R., & Wearing, A. (1991). Top-down versus bottom-up theories of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 81-100. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00292652

  • Hudson, N. W., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2020). The highs and lows of love: Romantic relationship quality moderates whether spending time with one’s partner predicts gains or losses in well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(4), 572-589. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219867960

  • Hutz, C. S., Zanon, C., & Bardagi, M. (2014). Satisfação de Vida. In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avaliação em Psicologia Positiva (pp.43-48). Porto Alegre, Brazil: Artmed.

  • Kamp Dush, C. M., & Amato, P. R. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), 607-627. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407505056438

  • Karney, B. R. (2015). Why marriages change over time. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, J. A. Simpson, & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 3 Interpersonal relations, (pp. 557–579). Washington, D.C., WA, USA: American Psychological Association.

  • Kim-Prieto, C., & Miller, L. (2018). Intersection of religion and subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 402-410). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Lansford, J. E. (2018). A lifespan perspective on subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being, (357–370). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132-140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132

  • Londero-Santos, A., Natividade, J. C., & Féres-Carneiro, T. (2021). Uma medida de satisfação com o relacionamento amoroso [A measure of relationship satisfaction]. Avaliação Psicológica, 20(1), 11-22. https://doi.org/10.15689/ap.2021.2001.18901.02

  • Londero-Santos, A., Pereira Neto, J. C., & Natividade, J. C. (2017, October). Satisfação conjugal e coping diádico como preditores do bem-estar subjetivo [Marital satisfaction and dyadic coping as predictors of subjective well-being]. Poster session presented at 47ª Reunião Anual da Sociedade Brasileira de Psicologia, São Paulo, Brazil.

  • Love, A. B., & Holder, M. D. (2016). Can romantic relationship quality mediate the relation between psychopathy and subjective well-being? Journal of Happiness Studies, 17(6), 2407-2429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9700-2

  • Lucas, R. E. (2007). Adaptation and the set-point model of subjective well-being: Does happiness change after major life events? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 75-79. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00479.x

  • Lucas, R. E. (2018). Exploring the associations between personality and subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 246 – 259). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Luhmann, M., Hofmann, W., Eid, M., & Lucas, R. E. (2012). Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 592-615. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025948

  • Malkoç, A. (2011). Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish sample. Procedia: Social and Behavioral Sciences, 12, 577-581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.02.070

  • Margelisch, K., Schneewind, K. A., Violette, J., & Perrig-Chiello, P. (2017). Marital stability, satisfaction and well-being in old age: variability and continuity in long-term continuously married older persons. Aging & Mental Health, 21(4), 389-398. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1102197

  • Mogilner, C., Whillans, A., & Norton, M. I. (2018). Time, money, and subjective well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 504-519). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Myers, D. G. (1999). Close relationships and quality of life. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 374–391). New York, NY, USA: Russell Sage Foundation.

  • Natividade, J. C., Barros, M. C., & Hutz, C. S. (2012). Influência da flexão de gênero dos adjetivos em instrumentos psicológicos em português [The influence of adjectives gender flexion in psychological instruments in portuguese]. Avaliação Psicológica, 11(2), 259-264. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2015.1.16901

  • Natividade, J. C., Carvalho, N. M., Londero-Santos, A., Carvalho, T. F., Santos, L. S., & Fagundes, L. S. (2019). Gratidão no Contexto Brasileiro: Mensuração e Relações com Personalidade e Bem-Estar [Gratitude in the Brazilian Context: Measurement and Relationship with Personality and Well-Being]. Avaliação Psicológica, 18(4), 400-410. https://doi.org/10.15689/ap.2019.1804.18712.08

  • Natividade, J. C., & Hutz, C. S. (2015). Escala reduzida de descritores dos cinco grandes fatores de personalidade: prós e contras [Short form scale of descriptors of the Five Personality Factors: Pros and cons]. PSICO, 46(1), 79-89. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2015.1.16901

  • Nelson-Coffey, S. K. (2018). Married...with children: The science of well-being in marriage and family life. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 410-424). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Nes, R., & Roysamb, E. (2015). The heritability of subjective well-being: review and meta-analysis. In M. Pluess (Ed.), Genetics of psychological well-being: The role of heritability and genetics in positive psychology (pp.75-96). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.

  • Newman, D. B., & Graham, J. (2018). Religion and well-being. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 30–40). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Noronha, A. P. P., Lamas, K. C. A., & Barros, M. V. C. (2016). Afetos e personalidade: suas relações em estudantes universitários [Affects and personality: Their relationships in college students]. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 18(2), 75-88. https://doi.org/10.15348/1980-6906/psicologia.v18n2p75-88

  • Noronha, A. P. P., Martins, D. F., Campos, R. R. F., & Mansão, C. S. M. (2015). Relações entre afetos positivos e negativos e os cinco fatores de personalidade [Relationship between positive and negative affects and the big five]. Estudos de Psicologia, 20(2), 92-101. https://doi.org/10.5935/1678-4669.20150011

  • Perelli-Harris, B., Hoherz, S., Lappegård, T., & Evans, A. (2019). Mind the “Happiness” gap: The relationship between cohabitation, marriage, and subjective well-being in the United Kingdom, Australia, Germany, and Norway. Demography, 56(4), 1219-1246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-019-00792-4

  • Proulx, C. M., Helms, H. M., & Buehler, C. (2007). Marital quality and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 69(3), 576-593. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00393.x

  • Rhoades, G. K., Kamp Dush, C. M., Atkins, D. C., Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. (2011). Breaking up is hard to do: The impact of unmarried relationship dissolution on mental health and life satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(3), 366-374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023627

  • Rusbult, C. E., Martz, O. J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The Investment Model Scale: Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size. Personal Relationships, 5(4), 357-387. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1998.tb00177.x

  • Schimmack, U. (2008). The structure of subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 97–123). New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being: Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York, NY, USA: Russell Sage Foundation.

  • Soto, C. J. (2015). Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and prospective relations of the big five with subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 83(1), 45-55. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12081

  • Steptoe, A., Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2015). Subjective well-being, health, and ageing. Lancet, 385(9968), 640-648. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61489-0

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2013). Subjective well-being and income Is There Any Evidence of Satiation? The American Economic Review, 103(3), 598-604. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.103.3.598

  • Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. S. (2006). Does marriage make people happy, or do happy people get married? Journal of Socio-Economics, 35(2), 326-347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2005.11.043

  • Suh, E. M., & Choi, S. (2018). Predictors of subjective well-being across cultures. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.), Handbook of well-being (pp. 795–806). Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers.

  • Tay, L., Li, M., Myers, D., & Diener, E. (2014). Religiosity and subjective well-being: An international perspective. In C. Kim-Prieto (Ed.), Cross-cultural advancements in positive psychology: Vol. 9. Religion and spirituality across cultures (pp. 163–175). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.

  • Ventegodt, S., Flensborg-Madsen, T., Andersen, N. J., & Merrick, J. (2008). Which factors determine our quality of life, health and ability? Results from a Danish population sample and the Copenhagen Perinatal Cohort. Journal of the College of Physicians and Surgeons--Pakistan, 18(7), 445-450.

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Ames, IA, USA: The University of Iowa.

  • Zanon, C., Bardagi, M. P., Layous, K., & Hutz, C. (2014). Validation of the satisfaction with life scale to brazilians: Evidences of measurement noninvariance across Brazil and US. Social Indicators Research, 119, 443-453. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-013-0478-5

  • Zanon, C., & Hutz, C. S. (2014). Escala de afetos positivos e negativos (PANAS). In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avaliação em Psicologia Positiva (pp. 63-69). Porto Alegre, Brazil: Artmed.